The taste of music

Zarlino, one of the most important music theorists of the XVI century, described the minor consonances as 'sweet' (dolci) and 'soft' (soavi) (Zarlino 1558/1983, in On theModes New Haven, CT: Yale University Press, 1983). Hector Berlioz, in his Treatise on Modern Instrumentation a...

Descripción completa

Guardado en:
Detalles Bibliográficos
Autores principales: Mesz, B., Trevisan, M.A., Sigman, M.
Formato: JOUR
Materias:
Acceso en línea:http://hdl.handle.net/20.500.12110/paper_03010066_v40_n2_p209_Mesz
Aporte de:
id todo:paper_03010066_v40_n2_p209_Mesz
record_format dspace
spelling todo:paper_03010066_v40_n2_p209_Mesz2023-10-03T15:18:11Z The taste of music Mesz, B. Trevisan, M.A. Sigman, M. adult analysis of variance article auditory stimulation female human male music perceptive discrimination taste Acoustic Stimulation Adult Analysis of Variance Discrimination (Psychology) Female Humans Male Music Taste Zarlino, one of the most important music theorists of the XVI century, described the minor consonances as 'sweet' (dolci) and 'soft' (soavi) (Zarlino 1558/1983, in On theModes New Haven, CT: Yale University Press, 1983). Hector Berlioz, in his Treatise on Modern Instrumentation and Orchestra- tion (London: Novello, 1855), speaks about the 'small acid-sweet voice' of the oboe. In line with this tradition of describing musical concepts in terms of taste words, recent empirical studies have found reliable associations between taste perception and low-level sound and musical parameters, like pitch and phonetic features. Here we investigated whether taste words elicited consistent musical representations by asking trained musicians to improvise on the basis of the four canonical taste words: sweet, sour, bitter, and salty. Our results showed that, even in free improvisation, taste words elicited very reliable and consistent musical patterns: 'bitter' improvisations are low-pitched and legato (without interruption between notes), 'salty' improvisations are staccato (notes sharply detached from each other), 'sour' improvisations are high-pitched and dissonant, and 'sweet' improvisations are consonant, slow, and soft. Interestingly, projections of the improvisations of taste words to musical space (a vector space defined by relevant musical parameters) revealed that, in musical space, improvisations based on different taste words were nearly orthogonal or opposite. Decoding methods could classify binary choices of improvisations (ie identify the improvisation word from the melody) at performance of around 80%öwell above chance. In a second experiment we investi- gated the mapping from perception of music to taste words. Fifty-seven non-musical experts listened to a fraction of the improvisations. We found that listeners classified with high performance the taste word which had elicited the improvisation. Our results, furthermore, show that associations of taste and music go beyond basic sensory attributes into the domain of semantics, and open a new venue of investigation to understand the origins of these consistent taste-musical patterns. © 2011 a Pion publication. Fil:Mesz, B. Universidad de Buenos Aires. Facultad de Ciencias Exactas y Naturales; Argentina. Fil:Trevisan, M.A. Universidad de Buenos Aires. Facultad de Ciencias Exactas y Naturales; Argentina. Fil:Sigman, M. Universidad de Buenos Aires. Facultad de Ciencias Exactas y Naturales; Argentina. JOUR info:eu-repo/semantics/openAccess http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.5/ar http://hdl.handle.net/20.500.12110/paper_03010066_v40_n2_p209_Mesz
institution Universidad de Buenos Aires
institution_str I-28
repository_str R-134
collection Biblioteca Digital - Facultad de Ciencias Exactas y Naturales (UBA)
topic adult
analysis of variance
article
auditory stimulation
female
human
male
music
perceptive discrimination
taste
Acoustic Stimulation
Adult
Analysis of Variance
Discrimination (Psychology)
Female
Humans
Male
Music
Taste
spellingShingle adult
analysis of variance
article
auditory stimulation
female
human
male
music
perceptive discrimination
taste
Acoustic Stimulation
Adult
Analysis of Variance
Discrimination (Psychology)
Female
Humans
Male
Music
Taste
Mesz, B.
Trevisan, M.A.
Sigman, M.
The taste of music
topic_facet adult
analysis of variance
article
auditory stimulation
female
human
male
music
perceptive discrimination
taste
Acoustic Stimulation
Adult
Analysis of Variance
Discrimination (Psychology)
Female
Humans
Male
Music
Taste
description Zarlino, one of the most important music theorists of the XVI century, described the minor consonances as 'sweet' (dolci) and 'soft' (soavi) (Zarlino 1558/1983, in On theModes New Haven, CT: Yale University Press, 1983). Hector Berlioz, in his Treatise on Modern Instrumentation and Orchestra- tion (London: Novello, 1855), speaks about the 'small acid-sweet voice' of the oboe. In line with this tradition of describing musical concepts in terms of taste words, recent empirical studies have found reliable associations between taste perception and low-level sound and musical parameters, like pitch and phonetic features. Here we investigated whether taste words elicited consistent musical representations by asking trained musicians to improvise on the basis of the four canonical taste words: sweet, sour, bitter, and salty. Our results showed that, even in free improvisation, taste words elicited very reliable and consistent musical patterns: 'bitter' improvisations are low-pitched and legato (without interruption between notes), 'salty' improvisations are staccato (notes sharply detached from each other), 'sour' improvisations are high-pitched and dissonant, and 'sweet' improvisations are consonant, slow, and soft. Interestingly, projections of the improvisations of taste words to musical space (a vector space defined by relevant musical parameters) revealed that, in musical space, improvisations based on different taste words were nearly orthogonal or opposite. Decoding methods could classify binary choices of improvisations (ie identify the improvisation word from the melody) at performance of around 80%öwell above chance. In a second experiment we investi- gated the mapping from perception of music to taste words. Fifty-seven non-musical experts listened to a fraction of the improvisations. We found that listeners classified with high performance the taste word which had elicited the improvisation. Our results, furthermore, show that associations of taste and music go beyond basic sensory attributes into the domain of semantics, and open a new venue of investigation to understand the origins of these consistent taste-musical patterns. © 2011 a Pion publication.
format JOUR
author Mesz, B.
Trevisan, M.A.
Sigman, M.
author_facet Mesz, B.
Trevisan, M.A.
Sigman, M.
author_sort Mesz, B.
title The taste of music
title_short The taste of music
title_full The taste of music
title_fullStr The taste of music
title_full_unstemmed The taste of music
title_sort taste of music
url http://hdl.handle.net/20.500.12110/paper_03010066_v40_n2_p209_Mesz
work_keys_str_mv AT meszb thetasteofmusic
AT trevisanma thetasteofmusic
AT sigmanm thetasteofmusic
AT meszb tasteofmusic
AT trevisanma tasteofmusic
AT sigmanm tasteofmusic
_version_ 1782030889748791296